Page 2, Memories of Sexual Betrayal

 

In addition, it is important to remember that people who have lived through a period of sexual victimization by a trusted caretaker have had to learn a kind of double-speak language (Orwell 1949). This point is also made by Wallen (1994), who says: "[For the incest victim,] pain felt by the body has been called pleasure. Violation has been called love. When the body has actually felt pleasure, or the heart love, [the] mind has felt betrayed, [and is] deeply ashamed for having failed to fully resist the encounter" (p. 38). Thus, as dissociated memories emerge, still in double-speak language, the adult has every reason to mistrust them, deny them, find them illogical, and feel both betraying and betrayed once again, this time by her or his own self.


The Current Research
The literature about childhood sexual abuse is growing so fast that it is difficult to keep current. In the past several years there have been special issues of the Journal of Applied Cognitive Psychology (1994, Vol 8, No. 4), The Journal of Interpersonal Violence (1993, Vol. 8, No. 3) and Psychoanalytic Inquiry (1992, Vol. 12, No. 1) devoted to the subject. Multiple important articles have also appeared in Psychoanalytic Psychology (Brenneis 1994a, Greer 1994, Lerner 1994), American Psychologist (Byrd 1994, Gleaves 1994, Gold et al. 1994, Loftus 1994, Olio 1994, Peterson 1994, and Pope 1996), and the Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association (Brenneis 1994b, Person and Klar 1994).

While I cannot review this literature in depth here, I would like to discuss briefly two lines of work that promise to add a great deal to our understanding of memories of sexual betrayal. One is about the biology of traumatic memory, and the other is about the prospective study of recall of childhood sexual abuse.

The psychobiology of traumatic memory recall has been of interest to a number of investigators, including Kolb (1987) and van der Kolk (1988, 1989, 1991, 1995; van der Kolk and Greenberg 1987; van der Kolk, McFarlane, and Weisaeth 1996). In a 1994 article in Nature, a group of California investigators (Cahill, Prins, Weber, and McGaugh) corroborate that traumatic memory is encoded differently than conventional memory in humans. They did this by administering propanolol to subjects being exposed to both neutral and traumatic stories. Propanolol is a beta-blocker, a drug that blocks the effects of adrenaline and noradrenaline, both of which are released during emotional arousal. The researchers found that the beta-blocker significantly impaired memory of the emotionally arousing story but not of the emotionally neutral story. Since the drug affected traumatic memory only, the study suggests a separate biological system for encoding and recalling emotional trauma. Specifically, the study supports the idea that enhanced memory for emotional experiences involves activation of the beta-andrenergic system. Blocking this activation therefore blocks traumatic and emotion-laden memory. To my knowledge, this study is thus the first reported experimental induction of the process of dissociation.

While retrospective studies have added to our understanding of traumatic memory, they are not as persuasive as prospective studies, in which subjects are followed over time rather than asked to recall their history. Linda Meyer Williams (1994) of the University of New Hampshire has done a longitudinal prospective study of the recall of women whose childhood sexual abuse was documented at the time in hospital records. Seventeen years later, she followed up on these girls and found that 38 percent of them recalled neither the abuse for which they had received medical attention at the time nor other molestations by the same abuser. This group of nonrecallers included women who were able to discuss with the researchers other incidents of sexual abuse in their lives with other abusers. When girls younger than seven were eliminated from the sample in order to control for the unreliability of early childhood memory, 28 percent still did not recall the abuse. Williams concludes that "forgetting is associated not only with age but with the relationship to the offender" (p. 1174, italics added). Girls abused at a very young age were most likely to forget, as were girls abused by someone they knew. Abuse by a stranger, however, was more likely to be remembered even when the study controlled for age, thus suggesting that "age-related, cognitive developmental theories are not sufficient explanation for memories of traumatic events" (p. 1174). Thus some women recalled abuse by a stranger that occurred even before age three, while others did not recall repeated abuse by a family member that occurred at much later ages. The Williams study, then, is a powerful support for the idea that sexual abuse trauma is often not remembered by the victim, particularly if it was incestuous and occurred at an early age.


What Does Contemporary Psychoanalysis Offer?
What do we as psychoanalysts and psychoanalytically oriented therapists uniquely have to contribute in our work with sexually abused patients? I believe the most important thing we offer is our training to tolerate and help our patients tolerate ambiguity. This training helps us and our patients to live with the uncertainty of not always knowing what is true, what we believe, or what fits together.

Writing in another context, Earl Witenberg (1978) wrote about the inevitability of uncertainty in psychoanalysis. He warned analysts not to confuse what they believe with what they know, saying, "When one transforms belief into knowledge, one does the patient an injustice. . . . Theories are always necessary to guide us, but there is a great deal of uncertainty in the practice [of psychoanalysis]. . . .[T]he hazard is that we cling to our beliefs . . . and explain the patient in terms of our theories and not attend to how or what the patient is saying" (pp. 278-279). When this is applied to the subject of this volume, Witenberg is sensibly warning us not to approach patients with preconceived ideas about whether sexual betrayal did or did not take place, but rather to allow the material to unfold from the patient without our prior judgment of it. Likewise, we must not allow our patient's or our own anxiety to rush us to conclusions about the veridicality or lack of it in emerging memories of sexual betrayal.


A Poet's Perspective
Before you read the papers on memory that constitute the remainder of this discourse, I want to conclude by reminding you what the playwright and poet Tennessee Williams had to say about it. As is so often true, in this case an artist captures in a few words qualities of human experience that psychoanalysts can only attempt to delineate in lengthy papers such as these.

In the opening monologue of The Glass Menagerie (1944), Williams' semi-autobiographical re-creation of his own traumatizing family, Tom Wingfield, the writer's alter ego and the play's narrator, says: "The play is memory. Being a memory play, it is dimly lighted, it is sentimental, it is not realistic" (p. 341). Williams thus lets us know immediately that memory distorts.

The question for us all to address here is whether these distortions do essential damage to truth. Anyone who sees The Glass Menagerie knows that distorted memory can highlight and communicate human truth. Any therapist who sits with a patient reaching for elusive memories of childhood sexual betrayal also knows that even distorted memories can lead to fundamental and healing psychological truths.

References

Alpert, J. L., Brown, L.S., Ceci, S., Courtois, C., Loftus, E., and Ornstein, P. (1994). Interim Report of the APA Working Group on Investigation of Memories of Childhood Abuse. Washington D. C.: American Psychological Association.
Aron, L. (1996). A Meeting of Minds: Mutuality in Psychoanalysis. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press.
_____ and Frankel, J. (1994). Who is overlooking whose reality? Commentary on Tabin's "Freud's shift from the seduction theory: Some overlooked reality factors." Psychoanalytic Psychology, 11:291-301.
_____ and _____ (1995). Response to Tabin. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 12:317-319.
Balint, M. (1968). The Basic Fault. London: Tavistock.
Bass, E., and Davis, L. (1988). The Courage to Heal. New York: Harper & Row.
Bolton, F., Morris, L., and MacEachron, A. (1990). Males at Risk: The Other Side of Child Sexual Abuse. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Brenneis, C.B. (1994a). Can early childhood trauma be reconstructed from dreams? On the relation of dreams to trauma. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 11:429-447.
_____ (1994b). Belief and suggestion in the recovery of memories of childhood sexual abuse. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 42:1027-1052.
Breuer, J., and Freud, S. (1893-1895). Studies on hysteria. Standard Edition, 2:1-306. London: Hogarth Press, 1962.
Briere, J. (1989). Therapy for Adults Molested as Children: Beyond Survival. New York: Springer Press.
Bromberg, p. (1991). On knowing one's patient inside out: The aesthetics of unconscious communication. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 1:399-422.
_____ (1993). Shadow and substance: A relational perspective on clinical process. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 10:147-168.
_____ (1994). "Speak! That I may see you": Some reflections on dissociation, reality, and psychoanalytic listening. Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 4: 517-547.
Byrd, K. (1994). The narrative reconstructions of incest survivors. American Psychologist, 49:439-440.
Cahill, L., Prins, B., Weber, M., and McGaugh, J. (1994). Beta-adrenergic activation and memory for emotional events. Nature, 371:702-704.
Cohler, B.J. (1994). Memory recovery and the use of the past: A commentary on Lindsay and Read from psychoanalytic perspectives. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8:365-378.
Courtois, C. (1988). Healing the Incest Wound. New York: Norton.
_____ (1992). The memory retrieval process in incest survivor therapy. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 1:15-31.
Crews, F. (1994/1995). The revenge of the repressed. The New York Review of Books. Part 1: November 17, 1994, pp. 54-60; Part 2: December 1, 1994, pp. 49-58. Reprinted (1995) in The Memory Wars: Freud's Legacy in Dispute. New York: A New York Review Book.
Davies, J. M., and Frawley, M. G. (1994). Treating the Adult Survivor of Childhood Sexual Abuse: A Psychoanalytic Perspective. New York: Basic Books.
Ehrenberg, D. (1987). Abuse and desire: a case of father-daughter incest. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 23:593-604.
_____ (1992). Abuse and desire. In The Intimate Edge, pp. 159-191. New York: Norton.
Fairbairn, R. (1954). Observations on the nature of hysterical states. British Journal of Medical Psychology. 27:105-125.
Ferenczi, S. (1932/1988). The Clinical Diary of Sandor Ferenczi, ed. J. Dupont (trans. M. Balint and N.Z. Jackson). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
_____ (1933/1988). Confusion of tongues between adults and the child. Reprinted in Contemporary Psychoanalysis 24:196-206.
Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child Sexual Abuse: New Theory and Research. New York: The Free Press.
_____, ed. (1986). A Sourcebook on Child Sexual Abuse. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Fortune, C. (1993). The case of "RN": Sandor Ferenczi's radical experiment in psychoanalysis. In: The Legacy of Sandor Ferenczi, ed. L. Aron & A. Harris, pp. 101-120. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Freedman, A.M., Kaplan, H.I., and Sadock B.J. (1976). Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
Freud, S. (1896). The aetiology of hysteria. Standard Edition, 3:189-221. London: Hogarth, 1962.
_____ (1900). The interpretation of dreams. Standard Edition, 4:entire, 5:1-630. London: Hogarth, 1962.
Ganaway, G. (1989). Historical versus narrative truth: Clarifying the role of exogenous trauma in the etiology of MPD and its variants. Dissociation, 2:205-220.
_____ (1992). On the nature of memories. Dissociation, 121-123.
Gardner, R.A. (1992). Belated realization of child sex abuse by an adult. Issues in Child Abuse Accusations, 4:177-195.
Gartner, R. B. (1993). Considerations in the psychoanalytic treatment of men who were sexually abused as children. Paper presented at the Spring meeting of Division 39 (Psychoanalysis) of the American Psychological Association, New York. Accepted for publication, Psychoanalytic Psychology.
_____ (1994). When the the analyst and the sexually abused patient are both men. Paper presented at the Spring meeting of Division 39 (Psychoanalysis) of the American Psychological Association, Washington, DC.
Gartner, R. B., and Goldman, D., with twenty-seven co-signers (1995). A reply to Frederick Crews. The New York Review of Books, January 12, pp. 42-43.
Gelinas, D. (1983). The persisting negative effects of incest. Psychiatry, 46:313-332.
Gleaves, D. (1994). On "The reality of repressed memories." American Psychologist, 49:440-441.
Gold, S., Hughes, D., and Hohnecker, L. (1994). Degrees of repression of sexual abuse memories. American Psychologist, 49:441-442.
Greer, J. M. (1994). "Return of the Repressed" in the analysis of an adult incest survivor: A case study and some tentative generalizations. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 11:545-561.
Herman, J. L. (1981). Father-Daughter Incest. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
_____ (1992). Trauma and Recovery. New York: Basic Books.
_____ and Schatzow, E. (1987). Recovery and verification of memories of
childhood sexual trauma. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 4:1-14.
Hidas, G. (1993). Flowing over -- Transference, countertransference, telepathy. In The Legacy of Sandor Ferenczi, ed. L. Aron and A. Harris, pp. 207-215. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Hunter, M., ed. (1990a). The Sexually Abused Male, vol. 1. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
_____, ed. (1990b). The Sexually Abused Male, vol. 2. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
_____ (1990c). Abused Boys: The Neglected Victims of Sexual Abuse. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Janet, p. (1898). Nevroses et idees fixes. Paris: Alcan.
Jones, E. (1957). The Life and Work of Sigmund Freud, vol. 3. New York: Basic Books.
Kluft, R. P., ed. (1990). Incest-Related Syndromes of Adult Psychopathology. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
Kolb, L. (1987). Neuropsychological hypothesis explaining post-traumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144:989-995.
Levine, H., ed. (1990). Adult Analysis and Childhood Sexual Abuse. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Lerner, p. (1994). Treatment issues in a case of possible multiple personality disorder. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 11:563-574.
Lew, M. (1988). Victims No Longer. New York: Harper & Row.
Lindsay, D. S., and Read, J. D. (1994). Psychotherapy and memories of child sexual abuse: A cognitive perspective. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8:281-338.
Lisak, D. (1993). Men as victims: Challenging cultural myths. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 6:577-580.
_____ (1994). The psychological consequences of childhood abuse: Content analysis of interviews with male survivors. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 7:525-548.
_____ (1995). Integrating a critique of gender in the treatment of male survivors of childhood abuse. Psychotherapy: 32:258-269.
_____, Hopper, J., and Song, p. (1996), The relationship between child abuse, gender adjustment and perpetration in men. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 9, 735-757.
_____ and Luster, L. (1994). Educational, occupational and relationship histories of men who were sexually and/or physically abused as children. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 7:507-523.
Loftus, E. (1992). When a lie becomes memory's truth: Memory distortion after exposure to misinformation. Current Direction in Psychological Science, 1:121-123.
_____ (1993a). Desperately seeking memories of the first few years of childhood: The reality of early memories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122:274-277.
_____ (1993b). The reality of repressed memories. American Psychologist, 48:518-537.
_____ (1994). The repressed memory controversy. American Psychologist, 49:443-445.
_____ and Ketcham, K. (1991). Witness for the Defense. New York: St. Martin's.
_____ and _____ (1994). The Myth of Repressed Memory. New York: St. Martin's.
Masson, J. (1984). The Assault on Truth. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.
Mendel, M. p. (1995). The Male Survivor. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Olio, K. (1994). Truth in memory. American Psychologist, 49:442-443.
Orwell, G. (1949). 1984. New York: Harcourt, Brace, Jovanich.
Peake, A. (1989). Issues of under-reporting: The sexual abuse of boys. Educational and Child Psychology, 6:42-50.
Person, E. S., and Klar, H. (1994). Establishing trauma: The difficulty distinguishing between memories and fantasies. Journal American Psychoanalytic Association, 42:1055-1081.
Peterson, R. (1994). Comment on Loftus. American Psychologist, 49:443.
Pope, K.S. (1996), Memory, abuse, and science: Questioning claims about the false memory syndrome epidemic. American Psychologist, 51:957-974.
Rachman, A. (1993). Ferenczi and sexuality. In: The Legacy of Sandor Ferenczi, ed. L. Aron and A. Harris, pp. 81-100. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Read, J. D., and Lindsay, D. S. (1994). Moving toward a middle ground on the "false memory debate": Reply to commentaries on Lindsay and Read. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 8:407-435.
Russell, D. (1983). The incidence and prevalence of intrafamilial and extrafamilial sexual abuse of female children. Child Abuse and Neglect, 7:133-146.
_____ (1984). Sexual Exploitation: Rape, Child Sexual Abuse, and Workplace Harassment. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
_____ (1986). The Secret Trauma: Incest in the Lives of Girls and Women. New York: Basic Books.
Sands, S. (1994). What is dissociated? Dissociation, 7:145-152.
Sgroi, S., ed. (1988). Vulnerable Populations, Volume 1. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
_____, ed. (1989). Vulnerable Populations, Volume 2. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Shapiro, S. (1993). Clara Thompson: Ferenczi's messenger with half a message. In: The Legacy of Sandor Ferenczi, ed. L. Aron and A. Harris, pp. 159-173. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
Shengold, L. (1989). Soul Murder. New York: Fawcett Columbine.
Simon, B. (1992). "Incest -- see under Oedipus complex": The history of an error in psychoanalysis. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 40:955-988.
Stern, D. (1983). Unformulated experience. Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 19:71-99.
Sullivan, H. S. (1956). Clinical Studies in Psychiatry. New York: Norton.
Tabin, J. K. (1993). Freud's shift from the seduction theory: Some overlooked reality factors. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 10:291-298.
_____ (1995). A bit more light on Ferenczi and Freud. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 12:305-315
Tavris, C. (1993). Beware the incest survivor machine. New York Times Book Review, January 3, 1993, pp. 1, 16-17
Urquiza, A., and Keating, L. M. (1990). The prevalence of sexual victimization in males. In The Sexually Abused Male, vol. 1, ed. M. Hunter. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books, pp. 89-104
van der Kolk, B. A., ed. (1987). Psychological Trauma. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
_____ (1988). The trauma spectrum: The interaction of biological and social events in the genesis of trauma response. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 1:273-290.
_____ (1989). The compulsion to repeat the trauma. Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 12:389-411.
_____ (1991). The biological response to psychic trauma: Mechanisms and treatment of intrusion and numbing. Anxiety Research, 4:199-212.
_____ (1995). The body, memory, and the psychobiology of trauma. In Sexual Abuse Recalled, ed. J.L. Alpert, pp. 29-60. Northvale, NJ: Jason Aronson.
_____ and Greenberg, M. S. (1987). Psychobiology of the trauma response: Hyperarousal, constriction, and addiction to trauma reexposure. In Psychological Trauma, ed. B. van der Kolk, pp. 63-88. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.
_____, McFarlane, A.C., and Weisaeth, L., ed. (1996). Traumatic Stress. New York: Guilford.
Wallen, R. (1994). Memory politics: The implications of healing from sexual abuse. Tikkun, 9:35-40.
Williams, L. M. (1994). Recall of childhood trauma: A prospective study of women's memories of child sexual abuse. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62:1167-1176.
Williams, Tennessee (1944). The Glass Menagerie. Reprinted in Twentieth Century Drama, ed. R. Cohn and B. Dukore, pp. 331-402. New York: Random House, 1966.
Witenberg, E. (1978). The inevitability of uncertainty. Journal of the American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 6:275-279.